Strabby Looks to Break Through the 2D Wall


A couple of weeks ago I mentioned the joy of discovering a new legion of expressive bloggers who were influenced by the experiences of “Stereo Sue”, one of whom is Sally Rye, aka “Strabby“.  In anticipation of breaking through the wall of her 2D flatland, Strabby had a nice epiphany that she blogged about yesterday.

Sharing that she was alarmed this past November 2010 when she “flunked” the Stereo Fly test, Strabby cited evidence that stereopsis exists on a 2D to 3D continuum, rather than being an all or nothing phenomenon.  Strabby suggests that the Stereo Fly should be part of every child’s visual screening.

Strabby has really hit on something here.  Our field has gravitated toward Random Dot Stereopsis testing, such as the “E” plates above.  If a patient has strabismus and is incapable of bifoveal fixtion, the two test plates are indistinguishable from one another.  Through the polaroid glasses, the “E” plate looks the same as the blank plate, and the appearance is the same whether the patient uses one eye or both eyes.  But that’s not how vision works in the real world.  In the real world, there is a continuum between true 2D vision with one eye closed, as compared to the sense of depth that a patient with strabismus might have with both eyes open.  Most strabismus patients in therapy find themselves on the weak end of the continuum prior to vision therapy, and acquire a greater sense of depth through therapy.

Aside from the many procedures that we employ in vision therapy to encourage greater spatial awareness, projection and depth, one of the targets I have come to enjoy is the distance stereo slide that is part of our Marco projection system.

The target has four pairs of lines arranged around a central cross.  Looking through polaroid filters, the patient sees one line of each pair with the right eye and the other line with the left eye.  Patients with “normal” binocular vision and good stereopsis have no trouble merging or fusing each pair of lines into one line.  The pair with the greatest separation or disparity, which is the one on the left, will create maximum stereoscopic effect and be perceived as closest to the patient.  The pair with the least separation, which is the top line, will be perceived as further away.

Now here’s the kicker:  patients with compromised binocular vision will say that they can perceive that all the lines are floating off the screen, but they can’t differentiate the relative depth among the four lines.  As they improve their stereoscopic localization through vision therapy, they are able to attain a greater sense of relative depth among the four lines.  The reason I like to use this as a test target during progress evaluations is that this isn’t a target that the patient has practiced during therapy sessions.  True perceptual learning has occurred when the patient is able to transfer what has been practiced to a novel target.  And for the strabismic patient, the payoff is how these disparity cues can be used in real space rather than in a simulated sense in a 2D booklet at arm’s length.

This is the 2D to 3D continuum to which Strabby is alluding.  Nicely done!

7 thoughts on “Strabby Looks to Break Through the 2D Wall

  1. Nice Len. Stereo has long been taught as either all or none. Indeed, high level binocularity requires that the brain “sees” the object in proper SIZE and SPACE. If one does SILO projection with polaroid or anaglyphs, you will find that very few “stereoscopically normal, 20 sec” persons have a distorted perception of where and how big a projected target is in space; either in crossed or uncrossed projection. I did a paper several years ago in the OVD about this “Quoits” projection. To me, when the patient approaches stereo projection in size and position, then they have stereopsis. There are indeed degrees of stereo.

    Regards, WC

  2. Len, Mistyped and hit sent too quickly—-What I meant to say was that the stereo normal persons almost always have a distorted projection. I did not mean to type that, “very few…have a distorted perception” but rather very few…have a proper projection”.

    WC

  3. Good point, Len. To rely on Randot testing is the miss the gradual increases in stereopsis that those with strabismus make. Even the Stereo Fly is not the place to begin. Holding a quoit, disparated against a dot pasted on the vectogram holder, at the centration point will pick up gross stere missed by other tests. It’s also an excellent place to begin therapy for stereo.

  4. I use the Quoit vectogram during inital testing, especially if the patient does not have Randot stereopsis. I recently had a constant alternating exotrope that could not perceive the stereo fly or Randot, but when I projected the quoitwith dot on the wall using a large 3rd degree target he was aware of the depth perception. He saw the 3 dimensional world for the first time. His depth perception wasn’t what he thought it would be, he thought things would jump out at him. I used the cover/uncover tecnique so that he could compare the difference with what he saw monocularly, and what he could see binocularly. It wasn’t big difference at first but after his second session it was better. I also use it for constant alternating esotropes, or esotropes in general to find a centration point if they have one and where we have to start in therapy. All in all, I have found the Quoit vectogram to be one of the best tools for awareness of Silo and for a beginning place for strabismic patients. But I like the test vectogram for progress checks. I think you should use a different target for testing than you use in therapy. Otherwise you are just teaching the test. Sharon

  5. Pingback: #6 it’s working!!! and SILO « strabby

  6. Pingback: Strabby Has Another Breakthrough « The VisionHelp Blog

  7. Pingback: blog-tacular! « strabby

Leave a comment